My opinion
 

By Dr. Ulysses P Albuquerque
Corresponding Author Dr. Ulysses P Albuquerque
Universidade Federal Rural de Pernambuco, - Brazil
Submitting Author Dr. Ulysses P Albuquerque
BIOETHICS

Ethic in Science, Plagiarism, Peer-Review Process.

Albuquerque UP. Scientific Misconduct. WebmedCentral BIOETHICS 2011;2(3):WMC001719
doi: 10.9754/journal.wmc.2011.001719
No
Click here
Submitted on: 09 Mar 2011 05:35:54 PM GMT
Published on: 10 Mar 2011 09:57:53 PM GMT

My opinion


Abstract: Plagiarism, fraud and falsification are issues modern scientists must confront. These problems themselves are not novel, but the discussion surrounding them has been revitalized due to recent, widely reported events. In order to keep the discussion of these topics active, and because misconduct is an important educational topic for all scientists, I will briefly discuss the current situation.
1. Introduction
The scandal surrounding the fraudulent publication of stem cell findings has been exhaustively discussed by the press, and the theme of scientific misconduct is now in the spotlight [1]. More than simply revealing that fraud, plagiarism and falsification of data occur, the primary effect of this event was to taint the public image of science. Scientists are commonly perceived as almost infallible, and news surrounding their misconduct, which is often disseminated in an alarmist or sensationalist manner, can contribute to a humanization of science and scientists.
We can wonder what would make a scientist deliberately behave in an unethical manner. Undoubtedly, the pressure to publish has increased in recent years. The well-known aphorism, “publish or perish,” was never so true as it is for modern scientists. It is necessary to publish in order to a) obtain research funds, b) progress professionally, and c) become recognized by peers. Competitiveness could be blamed for misconduct, but this would be a very simplistic argument [2]. In this article, I will examine the issues surrounding misconduct with the aim of contributing to the current debate. I will divide the topic into two questions: First, what are the types of scientific misconduct, and second, how frequent is misconduct?
2. What are the main types of misconduct?
Several behaviors can be considered scientific misconduct, and plagiarism is perhaps one of the most common transgressions. It is a common phenomenon and frequently committed unconsciously, which can occur when the plagiarist commits a serious ethical infraction while being unaware they are doing so. Chart 1 contains an adapted definition of plagiarism by Lucas [3], which will allow us to better clarify the extent of the problem.
Chart 1.  Definition of plagiarism, adapted from Lucas [3].
Global plagiarism – to steal a complete text from a single source and present it as if it were one’s own.
Plagiarism in remnants – to use ideas or languages from two or three sources and present them as one’s own.
Incremental plagiarism – to not give appropriate accreditation to specific parts of a text that were created by another person.
The first and third types of plagiarism listed above are unambiguous infractions, but the second type occurs frequently and is, mistakenly, often viewed as being encouraged. Plagiarism in remnants occurs when a writer, perhaps writing in another style, uses the structure of other texts to present his or her ideas. Many students use this strategy in an inappropriate way and justify their behavior based on the following advice from professors: “Use the structure of a given work as a reference”. The students interpret this advice as “Copy the structure”, when, in fact, the professor is asking them to observe what the author covered in that paragraph or text. A fourth type of plagiarism not mentioned by Lucas [3] is self-plagiarism, which occurs when one copies sections of one’s previous publications and republishes them in other journals. This concept applies to the introduction of an article or, more commonly, the Materials and Methods section. I have been astonished to learn how some students consider this a natural approach to drafting a manuscript. For example, I once heard a student, in an attempt to defend themselves from a reviewer, affirm innocently, “Are we not permitted to do that? I have seen several articles from a recognized group in which they publish the exact same introduction.”

I believe that as editors and reviewers, we cannot ignore this phenomenon. I say phenomenon because in each new generation of scientists there is a chance of misconduct occurring, whether out of deceitfulness or unawareness. Recent studies have shown that the severity and frequency of misconduct is underestimated by the scientific community [4, 5]. I believe that the small amount of attention given to the subject of misconduct is, curiously, part of a well-understood psychological phenomenon: denial. Denial is a psychological defense mechanism, and the admission of an uncomfortable fact can put important beliefs at risk (e.g., the integrity of science) and create huge anxiety. Nevertheless, statistics demonstrate the recurrence of misconduct as well the need to address the problem from both a preventative and educational standpoint.  Other types of misconduct are described in Chart 2.
Chart 2. Other types of scientific misconduct.

Creation: The deliberate creation of data or experimental circumstances and situations. In this case, the creative and imaginative researcher creates data or experiments. This type of misconduct is difficult to detect, especially if performed by an experienced scientist.
Falsification: The deliberate distortion or adjustment of data and results. For example, the scientist can omit values from a statistical analysis in order to adjust the result to fit the expected result. This can have serious consequences, especially in medical studies. Falsification is also very difficult to detect, even by experienced reviewers and editors.
Plagiarism: Plagiarism differs from other types of misconduct in that it is a misappropriation of the copyrights of another person. It is now a simple matter to detect plagiarism with the conveniences provided by the internet.
Self-plagiarism:  When an individual republishes exactly the same texts, graphics or images of a previous work with the intention of presenting them as novel.
3. How common is scientific misconduct?
Several authors have tried to answer this question (see [6]), most frequently using surveys and questionnaires. An obvious limitation of this method is that many people might not be willing to admit misconduct. It is perhaps easier to say that they have heard about misconduct or know somebody who has committed misconduct rather than acknowledging their own faults. Regardless of this limitation, the results of these studies are very interesting. In one of the most recent analyses, Fanelli [7] carried out a study on how frequently scientists fake or create data. The work of Fanelli [7] is the first meta-analysis to analyze several surveys taken on the issue of scientific misconduct. To summarize the findings, it was more common for scientists to admit to having changed data to improve results than to admit to having publishing admittedly falsified data. In addition, a significant number of scientists admitted to having observed misconduct among their peers. On average, 1.97% to 33.7% of scientists admitted to having created, falsified or modified data or to having behaved in a questionable way. Although misconduct is a sensitive subject, as are the techniques used to of evaluate scientists’ conduct, these data show that the problem exists.
4. Final considerations: is this a new witch-hunt?
Although I believe that educational and restrictive measures are necessary to avoid misconduct, it is necessary to be careful not to create a culture of distrust, to assume all researchers are guilty until the contrary can be proven. First, it is extremely difficult for editors and reviewers to detect certain types of misconduct; second, we cannot punish honest researchers. A lack of reflection and caution can lead to biased revisions and criticisms, based on suspicion rather than facts. Mutual trust is one of the fundamental qualities of professional relationships between scientists. The assumption of misconduct in published data would undoubtedly impact this relationship negatively. Conducting a witch-hunt, that is, reporting and disclosing the gravest cases of misconduct, can be productive and even necessary, especially when the data of a fraudulent publication impacts public policies. However, I believe many innocent individuals would be consumed in the effort to reveal these serious infractions. Therefore, I believe the best strategy to avoid scientific misconduct would be to educate and orient new scientists to their ethical responsibilities. This would be an educational strategy based on the understanding of science as a product of human intellect and on managing one’s career in such a manner that one can tolerate the pressure to publish without resorting to misconduct. Perhaps a better saying would be, “publish without perishing”!

Acknowledgement(s)


The author thanks Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico-CNPq (the Brazilian Research Council) for a productivity grant.

Reference(s)


1. Albuquerque, U.P.  The tyranny of the impact factor: why do we still want to be subjugated? Rodriguesia 2010, 61, 353-358.
2. Albuquerque, U.P. A qualidade das publicações científicas: considerações de um Editor de área ao final do mandato. Acta Botanica Brasilica 2009, 23, 292-296.
3. Lucas, S.E. The art of public speaking.  The McGraw-Hill Companies: New York, USA, 2003. 512p.
4. Long, T.C.; Errami, M.; George, A.C.; Sun, Z.; Gamer, H.R. Responding to possible plagiarism. Science 2009, 323, 1293-1294.
5. Errami, M.; Sun, Z.; Gerge, A.C.; Long, T.C.; Skinner, M.A.; Wren, J.D.; Garner, H.R. Identifying duplicate content using statistically improbable phrases. Bioinformatics 2010, 26,  1453-1457.
6. Marshall, E.  Scientific misconduct - How prevalent is fraud? That's a million-dollar question. Science  2000, 290,  1662–1663.
7. Fanelli, D. How many scientists fabricate and falsify research? A systematic review and meta-analysis of survey data. PLoS ONE 2009, 4(5), e5738. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0005738.

Source(s) of Funding


none

Competing Interests


none

Disclaimer


This article has been downloaded from WebmedCentral. With our unique author driven post publication peer review, contents posted on this web portal do not undergo any prepublication peer or editorial review. It is completely the responsibility of the authors to ensure not only scientific and ethical standards of the manuscript but also its grammatical accuracy. Authors must ensure that they obtain all the necessary permissions before submitting any information that requires obtaining a consent or approval from a third party. Authors should also ensure not to submit any information which they do not have the copyright of or of which they have transferred the copyrights to a third party.
Contents on WebmedCentral are purely for biomedical researchers and scientists. They are not meant to cater to the needs of an individual patient. The web portal or any content(s) therein is neither designed to support, nor replace, the relationship that exists between a patient/site visitor and his/her physician. Your use of the WebmedCentral site and its contents is entirely at your own risk. We do not take any responsibility for any harm that you may suffer or inflict on a third person by following the contents of this website.

Reviews
2 reviews posted so far

Previous Version:

Scientific Misconduct

Scientific Misconduct
Posted by Dr. Reinaldo F Lucena on 11 Apr 2011 01:09:55 PM GMT

Review of "Scientific Misconduct"
Posted by Dr. D. John Doyle on 08 Mar 2011 03:40:29 PM GMT

Comments
0 comments posted so far

Please use this functionality to flag objectionable, inappropriate, inaccurate, and offensive content to WebmedCentral Team and the authors.

 

Author Comments
0 comments posted so far

 

WebmedCentral Article: Scientific Misconduct

What is article Popularity?

Article popularity is calculated by considering the scores: age of the article
Popularity = (P - 1) / (T + 2)^1.5
Where
P : points is the sum of individual scores, which includes article Views, Downloads, Reviews, Comments and their weightage

Scores   Weightage
Views Points X 1
Download Points X 2
Comment Points X 5
Review Points X 10
Points= sum(Views Points + Download Points + Comment Points + Review Points)
T : time since submission in hours.
P is subtracted by 1 to negate submitter's vote.
Age factor is (time since submission in hours plus two) to the power of 1.5.factor.

How Article Quality Works?

For each article Authors/Readers, Reviewers and WMC Editors can review/rate the articles. These ratings are used to determine Feedback Scores.

In most cases, article receive ratings in the range of 0 to 10. We calculate average of all the ratings and consider it as article quality.

Quality=Average(Authors/Readers Ratings + Reviewers Ratings + WMC Editor Ratings)