Original Articles
 

By Mr. Richard Eckersley
Corresponding Author Mr. Richard Eckersley
National Centre for Epidemiology and Population Health, the Australian National University, - Australia
Submitting Author Mr. Richard Eckersley
PUBLIC HEALTH

Culture, Development, Mental Health, Population Health, Public Health, Social Determinants, Sustainability, Youth Health

Eckersley R. The Science and Politics of Population Health: Giving Health a Greater Role in Public Policy. WebmedCentral PUBLIC HEALTH 2011;2(3):WMC001697
doi: 10.9754/journal.wmc.2011.001697
No
Submitted on: 07 Mar 2011 06:51:06 AM GMT
Published on: 07 Mar 2011 06:19:51 PM GMT

Abstract


The dominant perspectives on population-health research and policy have significant gaps that are limiting public health’s role in public policy. Most public-health initiatives focus on individual risk factors associated with physical health. From a health perspective, this emphasis neglects the importance of mental health; from a prevention perspective, it under-estimates the importance of social and environmental determinants. Furthermore, social-determinants research focuses narrowly on socio-economic factors, notably inequality, to the neglect of cultural factors. Culture and mental health are closely linked. This is seen clearly in young people’s health, an important predictor of future population health. Contrary to longer historic trends and official perceptions, young people's health has arguably declined over recent generations in developed nations.

Acknowledging the importance of culture and mental health highlights the social significance of health in two ways: by casting new light on human development and national progress, and by showing health is an important social dynamic, a cause not just a consequence of social change. A broader view of the science and politics of population health would not only benefit health directly, it would allow public health to play a more influential role in public and political debate about national and global issues and priorities, including sustainable development, so having a more indirect, but ultimately powerful, effect on people’s health and wellbeing.

Introduction


The rise in life expectancy, which more than doubled globally last century, is a cornerstone of human development. While there are competing theories about what produced the health gains, they can be, broadly speaking, attributed to factors such as material advances, especially better nutrition; public-health interventions such as sanitation; social modernisation, including education and social welfare; and improved medical treatment and care (McMichael, 2001, pp. 193-201).

Historically, then, medicine and other health professions have been part of a broad, progressive movement that has improved not only life expectancy and health, but quality of life more broadly. The connection was close; the early emphasis in public health was on how social conditions influenced health and how they might be improved (Galea & Putnam, 2007). Today the relationship has changed. Health professions appear to be increasingly engaged in countering the growing harm to health of adverse social trends, at least in developed nations. At the same time, however, they have become part of the problem because of a scientific emphasis on, and political advocacy of, a biomedical model of health based on individual cases of disease, and their associated risk factors and treatments, at the expense of a social model of disease prevention and health promotion. This has contributed to a separation of population health from social conditions, to the detriment of both.

Criticisms of the biomedical model of health are not new (Galea & Putnam, 2007; Wade & Halligan, 2004). However, given the changing social context of health, they are more salient than ever. This paper seeks to bring together and develop these concerns so as to enlarge the debate about population health and its significance, both scientific and political. The paper argues that, from a health perspective, the science and politics of population health neglect mental health; from a prevention perspective they focus on individual risk factors to the neglect of social determinants, especially cultural factors, which are particularly important to mental health. These issues are illustrated by the patterns and trends in young people’s health and wellbeing. Better recognition of mental health and culture would also help to draw more attention to the role and importance of population health in human development and national and international affairs, including sustainable development.

To be clear about the paper’s intent, it is not saying these issues are not addressed in the health literature; the references (including to the author’s own work) make this obvious. Rather the paper argues that these issues are not adequately incorporated into the dominant scientific and political perspectives on population health. It links the issues to present a larger argument, set in the context of current health priorities and programs, about significant gaps in health research and policy, which, if addressed, could give health a larger role in public affairs and policy. The paper’s novelty lies in this breadth and scope.

These are complex matters, especially relevant to public health with its emphasis on prevention and populations. The patterns and trends in population health and their determinants and effects are often not clear and unambiguous. However, while this situation makes the evidence and the argument presented here contestable, and sometimes speculative, it also justifies challenging the orthodoxies that dominate considerations of population health.

The Neglect of Mental Health


Health is the result of a complex interaction of biological, psychological and social factors (the biopsychosocial model); and physical and mental health are closely interwoven and deeply interdependent (WHO, 2001). Medicine, however, continues to focus on the biological and neglect the psychosocial, despite the growing recognition of its importance to population health.

This artificial separation has been a formidable obstacle to understanding mental health (WHO, 2001); as a consequence, its importance to the wellbeing of individuals, communities and societies is underestimated. Both developing and developed countries show this bias towards physical health, and especially mortality. Developing countries tend to give priority in health to infectious disease and reproductive and child health; developed countries prioritise non-communicable diseases that cause early death (such as cancer and heart disease) over those that cause years lived with disability (such as mental disorders) (Prince et al., 2007).

Global action on chronic (physical) illness

The relative neglect of mental health is seen in the growing efforts in disease prevention and health promotion, both internationally and nationally. Recent initiatives, including the WHO global strategy for the prevention and control of non-communicable diseases (2008a), the Oxford Health Alliance (Darr et al., 2007) the Trust for America’s Health (2008) (in a report, ‘Prevention for a healthier America’),21 and the Australian National Preventative Health Taskforce (2009) (in its strategy paper, ‘Australia: the healthiest country by 2020’), all imply a wide health perspective, but focus on the physical diseases that contribute most to premature mortality, notably cardiovascular diseases, cancer, chronic respiratory diseases and diabetes. These diseases account for about 60% of all deaths globally.

The efforts will culminate in the United Nations’ first high-level meeting of the General Assembly on chronic non-communicable diseases in September 2011, billed as ‘a once in a generation opportunity to put chronic diseases on the global and national agendas’ (Beaglehole & Horton, 2010). These diseases have been ‘surprisingly neglected elements of the global-health agenda’ (Beaglehole et al., 2007) Mental illnesses, while also chronic, non-communicable diseases, are not part of this agenda, but are acknowledged to be ‘similarly ignored’.

The burden of mental illness

About 450 million people worldwide are suffering mental illness; only a small minority receives treatment (WHO, 2001). Worldwide, community-based studies have estimated the lifetime prevalence of mental disorders at 12%-49%, and 12-month prevalence at 8%-29% (WHO, 2008b). In 2004, neuropsychiatric conditions as a group accounted globally for 13.1% of the total burden of disease, measured as both death and disability (disability-adjusted life years or DALYs), the second largest contributor after infectious and parasitic diseases (WHO, 2008c). They account for about a third of the burden of disability, making them the most important source. Depressive disorders are the third largest specific cause of death and disability (and the largest in high- and middle-income countries), and are projected to become the leading cause by 2030. Yet the median allocation of the total health budget of nations to mental health is only 3.8% (WHO, 2008b).

The global burden of disease study has played a seminal part in exposing the importance of mental health to overall population health. However, its estimates of the burden of mental illness may still understate its significance for several reasons: mental disorders might affect many more people than the burden of disease estimates suggest, especially in middle- and low-income countries (Phillips et al., 2009); the estimates do not include the growing burden of suicide and self-inflicted injuries, which is counted under injuries (WHO, 2008c; Nock et al., 2008); the burden of mental disorders (in sharp contrast to chronic, physical diseases) falls mostly on those under 60 (WHO, 2008c), so increasing the personal, social and economic costs; and, finally, mental disorders increase the risk of physical diseases and injuries (Prince et al., 2007; WHO, 2008b; Moussavi et al., 2007), with one estimate that depressive disorders raise the risk of all-cause mortality by about 70% (Eaton et al., 2008) and affect adherence to treatment for other diseases (WHO, 2001; 2008b)

Aspects of this picture of mental health have been contested. For example, it has been argued that the high prevalence of mental disorders reflects changed DSM diagnostic criteria and the medicalisation of normal human emotions (Horwitz & Wakefield, 2007). This is part of a wider concern about the medicalising of life itself, and ‘disease mongering’: the selling of sickness that widens the boundaries of illness and grows the markets for those who sell and deliver treatments, including the medicalisation of health problems previously regarded as ‘troublesome inconveniences’ (Moynihan & Henry, 2006).

While medicalisation is undoubtedly occurring in the sense that new treatments are being developed for new conditions, this does not negate the core argument here that mental illness has been neglected relative to physical illness. The charge of ‘disease mongering’ applies to both physical and mental health, and is directed particularly at treatment provision. Indeed, it has been specifically associated with a policy priority of market-based economic development at the expense of more equitable social policies, such as public-health strategies (Moynihan & Henry, 2006). (Ironically, the medicalisation of mental health has contributed to greater awareness of its importance.)

Questions of definition, diagnosis and treatment aside, the disability associated with mental health problems is generally higher than for other chronic conditions (Eaton et al., 2008; Demyttenaere et al., 2004). Even mild cases cause levels of impairment equivalent to those associated with clinically significant, chronic physical disorders (Kessler et al., 2005). People attribute higher disability to mental disorders than to commonly occurring physical disorders, especially with respect to their ‘social and personal role functioning’ (with ‘productive role functioning’, the disability of mental and physical disorders is comparable) (Ormel et al., 2008). A comparison of the disability of 15 disease stages found severe depression ranked third behind quadriplegia and being in the final year of a terminal illness, and ahead of stroke and acute myocardial infarction (Schwarzinger et al., 2003).

Social determinants of health and the neglect of culture


Individual and population perspectives

The initiatives on chronic, physical ill-health, described above, emphasise modifying individual risk factors and lifestyles to improve population health, particularly tobacco use, poor diet and lack of physical activity, and harmful alcohol use. The broader perspective of the social determinants of health is largely absent. In promoting individual behavioural change in relation to specific risk factors, this orthodox public-health approach misses the crucial point that social conditions act on population health in ways that cannot be reduced to individual choices.

The two approaches – individual and social - are not completely separate and distinct; they represent the ends of a spectrum of interventions. Public-health programs, while they are directed at individual behaviour, aim to produce changes in the population as a whole. They range from public education to substantial legislative and regulatory changes to promote healthier living. And it can be argued that social conditions are addressed elsewhere in government: through taxation, welfare, consumer and worker safety, and environmental protection, for example. However, individual and population perspectives are conceptually different in important respects, and the effects of social conditions on population health need to be better understood and acknowledged as a basis for improving public policy and setting national goals and priorities.

Indeed, an important cost of the dominant individualised view of health is that governments, the media, the public, even health authorities and many health professionals, do not fully grasp a population-health perspective. As Rose observed, there is a relation between the mean of a characteristic in a population and the prevalence of the related disorder (Rose, 1992, pp. 64,72). ‘(T)he deviants are simply the tail of the population’s own distribution; they belong to each other…’ Or, as he said with specific reference to mental illness, ‘The visible part of the iceberg (prevalence) is a function of its total mass (the population average)’. Rose also argued that causes of cases can differ from causes of incidence: that is, explanations of individual differences in disease or disorder may be different from the explanations of population differences (Rose, 1992; Schwartz & Diez-Roux, 2001).

In other words, an individual’s lifestyle and health are socially conditioned; and efforts to improve health must match, or be appropriate to, the scale or level at which improvement is sought. It is difficult for people to make healthy choices when social conditions encourage unhealthy preferences. Changing their whole way of life is much more difficult than the specific changes on which public health has built its successes: not smoking, safe sex, and less drink-driving, for example. It puts people under considerable stress and they can react angrily to attempts by governments and others to interfere in their private lives and to tell them how to live (which is ironic given a vast media-marketing complex spends many billions of dollars a year on such ‘social engineering’, with little opposition).

Overlooking culture

In this public-health context, the final report of the WHO Commission on Social Determinants of Health (2008), which represents a culmination of several decades of research in this field, is to be welcomed for urging concerted global action on the more distal, social influences on health. However, while the Commission’s report, ‘Closing the gap in a generation’, is laudable in its specificity and detail, it is flawed in its conceptualisation of social determination. The focus is almost entirely on the socio-economic sources of health inequalities (or inequities), both within and between nations; this includes, within this context, factors such as urbanisation and economic growth. The report also reflects the orthodox emphasis on mortality reduction, the aim of all three of its targets for ‘closing the gap’.

The focus on inequalities might be understandable given their injustice, but it means the report overlooks other important social determinants. Environmental change (which is socially determined) is one category. The report mentions briefly environmental issues, especially climate change, noting the impacts are beyond its brief. This is defensible as environmental health has its own literature. Recent papers have strengthened the links between social and environmental determinants of health (not only climate change but also other problems such as biodiversity loss and resource depletion) (McMichael et al., 2008; Friel et al., 2008). However, the social frame remains socio-economics, especially poverty and inequality.

What is more puzzling is that the report says little about other determinants that are more obviously ‘social’ in nature, notably cultural factors. Like most of the recent literature on social determinants of health, the report acknowledges, in its conceptual framework, culture and social norms and values as important distal, or ‘upstream’, determinants – then says almost nothing about them. Its brief mention of cultural factors is limited to their being one aspect of the structural determinants of social hierarchy and inequity.

Wilkinson and Pickett’s best-selling and influential book, The spirit level: why equality is better for everyone (2009), is similarly restricted in its treatment of social determinants. The authors implicitly acknowledge the role of culture throughout the book in mentioning materialism, self-interest and consumerism, but when they deal explicitly with these issues - as in the reference (p 193) to 'a more individualistic economic philosophy or view of society', they discount them. Like the WHO Commission’s report, they believe inequality comes first and is a powerful influence on culture. They argue that ‘greater equality is the material foundation on which better social relations are built’, and social relations are, in turn, the basis of better health. There is little, if any, recognition that the reverse is also true: culture influences inequality (as well as many other aspects of human societies).

In this respect, both the report and the book are staying true to their dominant discipline. Epidemiology tends to regard culture mainly in categorical terms of class, subcultures, ethnicity or race, and so as a dimension of socio-economic status (Eckersley, 2001, 2006a). This approach ignores the much wider significance of culture as a system of meanings and symbols that defines how people see the world and their place in it, and gives meaning to personal and collective experience (Corin, 1995). Cultures define what can be known about the world, and so what can be done; in this sense they are fundamental to population health.

Some researchers have noted the need to push epidemiology beyond a narrow focus on social and economic relations, but this is not evident in the WHO Commission’s report or in The spirit level. As Glass (2006) has observed, any discussion of culture is strikingly unorthodox and counter-paradigmatic in epidemiology, having no place in its ‘Newtonian vision of cause and effect’ that involves what touches or invades the individual. ‘The idea of a symbolic field, permeating the thoughts and actions of a whole population, is simply fantastical and mysterious.’

Yet the effect on health of culture, in this broader sense of the dominant or defining belief system of a society, is attracting growing attention in the health literature (Corin, 1994, 1995; Eckersley, 2001, 2005, 2006a, 2007a; Carlisle & Hanlon, 2007; Hanlon & Carlisle, 2009; Carlisle et al., 2009). It is also examined in other disciplines such as psychology, sociology and anthropology. However, each discipline defines and conceptualises culture differently, making it a contentious subject; these complexities are discussed elsewhere (Eckersley, 2006a, 2007a). This paper summarises and updates the case for culture to challenge – again – the continuing, limited approach to social determination. It focuses on the health impacts of two defining qualities of modern Western culture: individualism and materialism. Thus the emphasis is on developed nations, although the issues are also of growing relevance to the developing world as a result of globalisation and modernisation.

Materialism means giving importance or priority to money and possessions. Individualism refers to the relaxation of social ties and regulation and the belief that individuals are independent of each other. Together, they form the core of modern consumer culture in that they focus attention on personal gratification and the satisfaction of individual needs and wants. Consumerism has had social benefits, including to health. However, its costs have increased as it gathered pace in the period after World War II - especially with the cultural liberalisation since the 1960s and the economic liberalisation from the 1980s – producing a culture of ‘hyper-consumerism’ that penetrates deeper into people’s lives.

The impacts of these cultural changes on physical health are apparent in activity- and diet-related physical problems such as obesity and diabetes, but are arguably greater on mental health. Indeed, culture and mental health are closely linked in that both have to do with what people think and feel; the neglect of one is associated with the neglect of the other (although even discussion of the social contributors to mental illness focuses on structural determinants such as poverty, urbanisation and technological change (WHO, 2001).).

A recent survey by the Joseph Rowntree Foundation (2008) illustrates the importance of culture in assessing social conditions. In a public consultation on today’s ‘social evils’, the Foundation revealed ‘a strong sense of unease about some of the changes shaping British society’. The top concerns were: a decline in community; individualism, consumerism and greed; and a decline in values. Poverty and inequality were one of six more concrete consequences, which also included the decline of the family, young people (as victims or perpetrators), drugs and alcohol, immigration, and crime and violence. These findings are consistent with those of surveys in other Western nations (Eckersley, 2005, pp.105-125, 2006b, 2009a).

While respondents in the Rowntree survey emphasised personal responsibility for social evils, they also believed some were ‘embedded in current ways of living and thinking’ and that bad choices and damaging behaviour could be symptoms of underlying social problems. Government, media, big business and religion were blamed for the evils. In a commentary on the survey, Bauman (2008) notes that social ills have their source in today’s ‘individualised society of consumers’, with consuming more being the ‘sole road to inclusion’, and ‘existential uncertainty’ now a universal human condition. Single-issue solutions might bring temporary and partial relief, he says, but short of reforming the individualistic way of life, they would not remove the cause.

Culture and young people’s health

The importance of culture and mental health, and the link between them, are evident in the health of young people, who best reflect the characteristics of the times because they are growing up in them. Their health is also an important predictor of future population health. Many of the attitudes and behaviours, and even the illnesses, that largely determine adult health have their origins in early life.

Of the almost half of Americans who suffer a clinical mental disorder in their lifetime, half experience the first onset by age 14 and three-quarters by age 24 (Kessler, 2005). WHO (2001) notes that child and adolescent mental disorders are very costly to society in both human and financial terms as many of these disorders can be precursors to much more disabling disorders during later life. Parental depression is a predictor of depression in children and adolescents, raising the prospect of the intergenerational transmission of risk for mental disorders, so creating a ‘vicious cycle’ of rising prevalence (Collishaw, 2009).

Contrary to the orthodoxy that young people’s health is continuing to improve in line with historic trends, it is arguable that it has declined in the developed world (with implications for developing regions) (Eckersley, 2008a, 2009b, 2011, in press). This situation partly reflects chronic, physical conditions, especially those associated with increasing obesity, which have led to predictions of a decline in life expectancy (National Preventative Health Taskforce, 2009). However, it rests more on the importance of the burden of mental illness in youth. Young adults experience higher rates of mental disorders than older adults, and these make up by far the largest contribution to their disease burden in developed nations (for example, accounting for 49% of the total burden in Australians aged 15-24) (AIHW, 2007).

Mental disorders appear to have increased markedly in prevalence among the young in Western nations in the second half of the 20th century, although the evidence remains contested (Eckersley, 2008a, 2009b; Collishaw, 2009). To cite just two recent studies, a meta-analysis of a widely used psychological inventory, the MMPI, found a steady decline in the mental health of American college students between 1938 and 2007 and high-school students between 1951 and 2002 (Twenge et al., 2010). Five to eight times as many college students in 2007 scored above common cut-off levels for psychopathology on at least one clinical scale as they did in 1938. The second study found considerably higher rates of emotional problems among English adolescents in 2006 than in 1986 (Collishaw et al., 2010). The more severe the reported symptoms, the larger the increase.

Socio-economic factors, such as social class and family structure, are not the main drivers of the patterns and trends in youth mental health (Eckersley, 2008a, 2009b; Collishaw et al., 2010; Sweeting et al., 2010; Twenge et al., 2010). Some studies show no socio-economic differences in the prevalence of mental health problems among youth and some even higher rates among the rich; other research has found increasing rates have occurred in all socio-economic groups and family types. The causes appear to be more existential and relational than material and structural, linked to factors that are associated with rising materialism and individualism. For example, the MMPI study favoured a cultural, not economic, explanation for the rise in mental ill-health, especially a shift towards extrinsic values and goals such as status and money (Twenge et al., 2010). An analysis of youth suicide rates, which rose in many Western nations in the second half of the 20th Century, found strong correlations with several measures of individualism (Eckersley & Dear, 2002).

Materialism and individualism reduce social support and personal control, both of which are crucial to health and wellbeing, through effects such as a heightened sense of risk, uncertainty and insecurity; a lack of clear frames of reference; a shift from intrinsic to extrinsic values and goals; increased, even unrealistic, expectations; an excess of freedom and choice; and the construal of the self as independent and separate from others (Eckersley, 2006a, 2008a, 2009b).

Britain’s Children’s Society report on childhood deals with the experiences of children in general rather than the problems of disadvantaged groups because, it says, ‘the world in which most children grow up is more difficult than it should be’ (Layard & Dunn, 2009, p. 11). It identifies the fundamental cause as ‘excessive individualism’, which holds that ‘our main duty is to make the most of ourselves’ and to be ‘as successful as possible’, in what has becomes ‘a struggle of each against all’ (p. 162).

There are other possible pathways to mental (and physical) disorders that are also associated with individualism and materialism, blended with other aspects of modernisation. These include changes in: the family and education (Collishaw, 2009; Eckersley, in press), the media and communication technologies (Eckersley, 2005, pp. 126-146; ACMA, 2007), religion and spirituality (Eckersley, 2007b; Williams & Sternthal, 2007), diet (Parker et al., 2006; Oddy et al., 2009; Jacka et al., 2010; Freeman, 2010), residential mobility (Oishi & Schimmack, 2010), social relationships and isolation (Holt-Lunstad et al., 2010; Mental Health Foundation, 2010), and exposure to environmental contaminants (Corvalan et al., 2005; Grandjean & Landrigan, 2006).

Thus a central feature of the changed patterns and trends in youth health over several generations is a shift in emphasis from socio-economic causes of ill-health to cultural; from material and economic deprivation to psychosocial deprivation; from a problem of material scarcity to one of excess. With this has come a shift in significance from physical health to mental health.

A poverty of ends, not means

Widening the lens of social determination has profound implications for population health. Addressing poverty and inequality is important from a health perspective. However, contrary to the prevailing social-determinants (and social-justice) orthodoxy, the core social challenge is not primarily a poverty of the means to the end of ‘the good life’ as it is currently defined and pursued; it is a poverty of the end itself. In other words, giving the disadvantaged and marginalised the opportunities and privileges of the majority, however much it will help them, will not solve the problems of population health; nor will reducing inequality.

This is especially true in the developed world, but it also applies to developing countries such as China and India, which have a rapidly growing middle class that is increasingly susceptible to diseases of affluence and modernity. Worldwide there are over 1.3 billion people who are overweight and about 800 million who are underweight, and the numbers are diverging rapidly (Popkin, 2007).

This is not to dismiss or diminish the needs of the poor or the costs of inequality. Indeed, given their persistence, it suggests they may be better addressed through acting on a broader conceptual framework that pays more attention to cultures, worldviews, and ideologies. To take one example: it is true that climate change will impact most on the poor of the world. But it is the world’s rich who are contributing disproportionately to its emergence as a global threat to population health. In other words, while the poor will bear most of the health burden of climate change, the determinants of this burden include not just poverty, but affluence.

This broader, social perspective shows that material progress does not simply and straightforwardly make people richer, so giving them the freedom to live as they wish. Rather, it comes with an array of cultural and moral prerequisites and consequences (for example, giving priority to money and what it buys) that affects profoundly how people think of the world and themselves, and so the choices they make. The pursuit of material progress, of evermore enrichment, as a cultural (and political) priority is jeopardising global health in ways that go well beyond its impact on poverty and inequality. This situation amounts to ‘cultural fraud’, in which the promotion of images and ideals of ‘the good life’ serve the demands of the economy, but do not reflect social realities or meet psychological needs (Eckersley, 2006a).

Population health in human development and as a social dynamic


Rethinking population health – giving due weight to mental health and due acknowledgement of social and cultural determinants – has important conceptual applications, including how societies think of human progress and development, and the need to consider health as a cause, not just a consequence or outcome, of changing social conditions.

Health in human development

The orthodox view of national progress and human development places Western nations at its leading edge. This might seem axiomatic. Not only are the people of Western liberal democracies, generally speaking, the richest and longest-lived, they do best on other common measures: happiness and satisfaction, freedom, education, governance and human rights (Inglehart, 2000; Helliwell, 2003, 2008; Inglehart et al., 2008; Eckersley, 2009a). Yet these measures provide an incomplete assessment of development.

Life expectancy is the main summary measure of population health. While this might have been valid in the past, it is now questionable: life expectancy, being based on mortality rates, does not reflect the growing importance to overall health and wellbeing of non-fatal, chronic illness, especially mental disorders, as discussed above. Similarly, happiness and life satisfaction, which have become increasingly popular in the past decade in assessing nations, do not reflect all aspects of wellbeing and all desirable psychological or social qualities, and may not, in any case, be comparable across cultures (Eckersley, 2009a, 2009c).

For all the positive qualities of Western societies, most of their people do not believe life is getting better (Eckersley, 2005, pp. 105-125, 2006b, 2009a). In contrast to the high levels of personal happiness and life satisfaction, many studies over the past few decades have revealed growing anger and anxiety about the changes in Western societies, as reflected in the findings of the Joseph Rowntree Foundation (2008). The concerns include excessive greed and selfishness, consumerism, too much competition and too little compassion, the loss of community, growing pressures on families, and drugs, crime and violence. There is a common perception that with individual freedom and material abundance, people don’t seem to know where to stop, or now have too much of a good thing.

However important the other major measures of progress may be, they do not get close enough to people’s lives to define and describe the determinants of health in its broadest sense (Eckersley, 2009a, 2009c). Income, democracy, human rights and education do not ‘measure’ the social concerns cited above, nor reflect the importance of the more intimate aspects of life, especially personal relationships, which are so important to health. The internal, psychosocial dynamics of Western societies reveal a very different picture of their ‘development’; in some respects, they could be societies in decline. For example, young people have the most to gain – and lose – from how well progress is defined and measured; if their health and wellbeing are not improving, it is hard to claim life overall is getting better.

The two views of human development could not be more different. The orthodox model emphasises what can easily be measured and the correlations between them: simple measures of wellbeing (happiness or life expectancy) and mainly material, structural and institutional factors. The psychosocial-dynamics model includes multiple measures of health and wellbeing and broader, cultural, moral and spiritual causes and correlates. The orthodox model may be useful in evaluating earlier stages of human development, but seems less relevant to so-called highly developed societies. Across all stages, but especially in the latter cases, it needs to be supplemented by a psychosocial-dynamics model.

To a significant extent, conventional indicators and models of development are measuring Westernisation or modernisation, not human development. While the concepts may overlap, they are not the same thing; Westernisation, for all its benefits, includes costs to wellbeing that the indicators are missing. At best, the qualities being measured may be desirable, even necessary, but are not sufficient. At worst, the benefits of qualities such as materialism and individualism are being counted, but not their costs, which are formidable and growing (and include social, economic and environmental impacts.)

This tension or contradiction is seen clearly with individualism. International comparisons suggest individual freedom is a major component of positive development, yet studies of its role in health tell a different story, as already discussed. The ambiguities and complexities of the effects of individualism are well expressed in the writings of social theorists, who recognise that the freedom people now have is both exhilarating and disturbing, and that new opportunities for personal experience and growth also create risks of social dislocation and isolation (Eckersley, 2006a, 2007a). Selznick (1992, pp. 7,8), for example, argues that modernisation initially brings the benefits of greater freedom, increased equality of opportunity, efficiency and accountability, and the rule of law – but at the price of ‘cultural attenuation’, in which, eventually, ‘selfhood itself become problematic’.

Health as a social dynamic

The importance of getting right the measures and models of human development is underscored by another neglected attribute of population health: it is an important dynamic in national and international affairs (Eckersley, 2010). Typically, public-health reports express this role in terms of the direct and indirect economic costs of poor health (that is, the costs of health care and lost productivity), with some acknowledgement of the social costs to individuals, families and communities (Darr et al., 2007; National Preventative Health Taskforce, 2009, Beddington et al., 2008). But these effects are just one part of a bigger, more complex, picture.

Poor health, both physical and mental, affects people in many life roles – as students, workers, parents and citizens. These impacts are not only the result of clinically significant health problems (which, nonetheless, affect substantial segments of the population). As Rose’s work (1992) implies, high rates of illness, especially mental illness, also reflect public mood, morale and vitality. Poor population health weakens a society's confidence and resilience, and so its capacity to deal with the challenges of the modern world (Eckersley, 2010). And this, in turn, further impacts on population health.

This is not widely appreciated. A false dichotomy often characterises debate and discussion about national and international affairs. On the one hand, these matters are seen as shaped by large, external forces such as economic development, technological change, environmental degradation and resource depletion, and war and conflict. Population health may be affected by these forces, but health itself is not usually seen as a contributor to larger-scale social developments. The perspectives of economics, politics and the environment dominate the discourse. On the other hand, considerations of health focus on internal, psychological and physiological processes and personal attributes, circumstances, behaviours and experiences. The dominant frame of reference is the biomedical model of health as an attribute or property of individuals, as discussed above.

This separation is misleading. The reality is that change in both social and personal, external and internal, worlds is shaped by a complex interplay between them. Understanding this interplay is important to comprehending what is happening in both realms. In other words, human ‘subjectivity’ plays an important part in the functioning of social systems; it is what most distinguishes them from other, biophysical systems. Health is not just an individual illness that requires treatment, but also an issue having national, even global, causes and consequences.

Health is a way of better understanding humanity and how people should live. Just as someone who is unwell will be less able to function effectively and withstand adversity, so too will a less healthy population make a less resilient society. Population health may be an important factor in determining whether societies respond effectively to adversity – or in ways that make the situation worse (Eckersley, 2010). In particular, mental health and morale could have a critical bearing on how societies cope with climate change and other 21st Century global threats (Eckersley, 2008b).

Population health perspectives can make an important contribution to sustainable development and the quest for a high, equitable and enduring quality of life: they provide a means of integrating, balancing and reconciling different social priorities by allowing them to be measured against a common goal or benchmark: improving human health and wellbeing (Eckersley, 2005, pp. 229-251, 2006b). Population health is, then, a key element of achieving a socially, economically and environmentally sustainable way of living - humanity’s greatest challenge.

Conclusion


As the biomedical dominance of health has increased, the social perspective has receded. Amongst the consequences, this paper has argued, mental health has been under-estimated as a component of population health, and social factors, especially cultural influences, have been neglected as determinants of health. A wider, more comprehensive, view of health could contribute to a better understanding of human development and of health as a social dynamic, a cause of social changes and developments, not just a consequence.

The current dominant perspective suits business and government. It is in biomedicine that profits are to be made, not in social health. The biomedical model also limits the political significance of health to the politics of healthcare services. This policy focus is challenging enough as governments struggle with rising demand and costs (in OECD countries health expenditure rose from an average 9.6% of GDP to 11.1% between 1995 and 2005 (AIHW, 2008), even as GDP itself increased substantially). However, the challenge is easy compared with trying to reconcile emerging health-based social realities with existing wealth-based political priorities. Embedded in the biomedical model is a disguised ideology that defends and promotes the status quo.

The scientific and political responses to the situation outlined in this paper might include more research on public and mental health, especially transdisciplinary approaches that integrate epidemiological, sociological, psychological and anthropological concepts and evidence. Similarly, with health services and programs, the share of the health budget allocated to public health and mental health should be increased. The response also needs to go beyond the health system to embrace, for example, rethinking the role and purpose of education, and greater regulation and control of business, especially advertising and marketing, the dominant promoters of consumerism.

However, the most important application of this perspective may be in the contribution it can make to a much broader political and public debate about the lives people want to lead, the societies they want to live in, and the futures they want to create. That debate is intensifying, but population health plays only a limited part in it.

This paper seeks to change this situation. In essence, it argues that a broad view of population health and its social determinants – socio-economic, cultural and environmental - challenges the legitimacy of the dominant worldview or paradigm of material progress, and supports the alternative, sustainable development. The contest between the two models, or narratives, of progress has been framed largely in economic and environmental terms, and the social dimension has been neglected. Population-health research can help to correct this distortion.

Medicine and other health professions might consider their purview is the provision of healthcare services. However, they have a powerful influence over the way society thinks about health, and acts on it; they provide the main reference points on health for government, media and public. It is time they reappraised more deeply the science and politics of population health; and it is appropriate that public health takes the lead in this task.

References


1. ACMA. (2007). Media and communications in Australian families 2007. Review of research literature. Australian Communications and Media Authority www.acma.gov.au/webwr/_assets/main/lib101058/maciaf2007_review_research_literature.pdf . Accessed September 15, 2010.
2. AIHW. (2007). Young Australians: their health and wellbeing 2007. Cat no PHE 87. Canberra: Australian Institute of Health and Welfare.
3. AIHW. (2008). Australia’s health 2008. Cat. no. AUS 99. Canberra: Australian Institute of Health and Welfare.
4. Bauman Z. (2008). The absence of society. Viewpoint. The social evils series. York: Joseph Rowntree Foundation; October. http://www.jrf.org.uk/publications/absence-society. Accessed June 20, 2009.
5. Beaglehole, R., & Horton R. (2010). Chronic diseases: global action must match global evidence. Lancet, 376, 1619-1620.
6. Beaglehole, R., Ebrahim, S., Reddy, S., et al. on behalf of the Chronic Disease Action Group. (2007). Prevention of chronic diseases: a call to action. Lancet, 370, 2152-2157.
7. Beddington, J., Cooper, C. L., Field, J., et al. (2008). The mental wealth of nations. Nature, 455, 1057-1060.
8. Carlisle, S., & Hanlon, P. (2007). Wellbeing and consumer culture: a different kind of public health problem? Health Promotion International, 22, 261-268.
9. Carlisle, S., Henderson, G., & Hanlon, P. W. (2009). ‘Wellbeing’: a collateral casualty of modernity? Social Science & Medicine, 69, 1556-1560.
10. Collishaw, S. (2009). Trends in adolescent depression: a review of the evidence. Depression in Childhood and Adolescence, ACAMH Occasional Papers 28, Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health (UK).
11. Collishaw, S., Maughan, B., Natarajan, L., & Pickles, A. (2010). Trends in adolescent emotional problems in England: a comparison of two national cohorts twenty years apart. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 885-894.
12. Corin, E. (1994). The social and cultural matrix of health and disease. In R. G. Evans, M. L. Barer, & T. R. Marmor (Eds.), Why are some people healthy and others not? The determinants of health of populations (pp. 93-132). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
13. Corin, E. (1995). The cultural frame: context and meaning in the construction of health. In B. C. Amick, S. Levine, A. R. Tarlov, & D. Chapman Walsh (Eds.), Society and health (pp. 272-304). New York: Oxford University Press.
14. Corvalan, C., Hales, S., & McMichael, A., et al. (2005). Ecosystems and human wellbeing: health synthesis. A report of Millennium Ecosystem Assessment. Geneva: World Health Organization. http://www.who.int/globalchange/ecosystems/ecosys.pdf . Accessed September 15, 2010.
15. Daar, A. S., Singer, P. A., Persad, D. L., et al. (2007). Grand challenges in chronic noncommunicable diseases. Nature, 450, 494-496. See also: http://www.oxha.org/. Accessed June 20, 2009.
16. Demyttenaere, K., Bruffaerts, R., Posada-Villa, J., Gasquet, I., et al. (2004). Prevalence, severity and unmet need for treatment of mental disorders in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys. JAMA, 291, 2581-2591.
17. Eaton, W. W., Martins, S. S, Nestadt, G., Bienvenu, O.J., Clarke, D., & Alexandre, P. (2008). The burden of mental disorders. Epidemiologic Review, 30, 1-14.
18. Eckersley, R. (2001). Culture, health and well-being. In R. Eckersley, J. Dixon, & B. Douglas (Eds.), The social origins of health and well-being (pp. 51-70). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
19. Eckersley, R. (2005). Well & good: morality, meaning and happiness. 2nd ed. Melbourne: Text Publishing.
20. Eckersley, R. (2006a). Is modern Western culture a health hazard? International Journal of Epidemiology, 35, 252-258.
21. Eckersley, R. (2006b) Progress, sustainability and human wellbeing: is a new worldview emerging? International Journal of Innovation and Sustainable Development, 1, 304-317.
22. Eckersley, R. (2007a). Culture. In S. Galea (Ed.), Macrosocial determinants of population health (pp. 193-209). New York: Springer.
23. Eckersley, R. (2007b). Culture, spirituality, religion and health: looking at the big picture. Medical Journal of Australia, 186 (10) (suppl), S54-S56.
24. Eckersley, R. (2008a). Never better – or getting worse? The health and wellbeing of young Australians. Canberra: Australia 21 Ltd. http://www.australia21.org.au/pdf/A21%20youth%20health%20&%20wellbeing.pdf. Accessed June 20, 2009.
25. Eckersley, R. (2008b). Nihilism, fundamentalism, or activism: three responses to fears of the Apocalypse. Futurist, 42 (1), 35-39.
26. Eckersley, R. (2009a). Population measures of subjective wellbeing: how useful are they? Social Indicators Research, 94, 1-12.
27. Eckersley, R. (2009b). The health and well-being of young Australians: patterns, trends, explanations and responses. In D. L. Bennett, S. J. Towns, E. L. Elliott, & J. Merrick (Eds.), Challenges in adolescent health: an Australian perspective (pp. 3-19). New York: Nova Science.
28. Eckersley, R. (2009c) Is the West really the best? Measuring the Progress of Societies. Issue 5, May (pp. 9-11). Paris: OECD. http://www.oecd.org/dataoecd/4/59/42836348.pdf. Accessed June 20, 2009.
29. Eckersley, R. (2010). Population health: a forgotten dimension of social resilience. In S. Cork (Ed.), Resilience and transformation: preparing Australia for uncertain futures (pp. 115-120). Melbourne: CSIRO Publishing
30. Eckersley, R. (2011). Troubled youth: an island of misery in an ocean of happiness, or the tip of an iceberg of suffering? Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 5 (suppl 1), 6-11.
31. Eckersley, R. (in press). A new narrative of young people’s health and wellbeing. Journal of Youth Studies.
32.Eckersley, R., & Dear, K. (2002). Cultural correlates of youth suicide. Social Science & Medicine, 55,  1891-1904.
33. Freeman, M. P. (2010). Nutrition and psychiatry [editorial]. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(3), 244-247.
34. Friel, S., Marmot, M., McMichael, A. J., Kjellstrom, T., & Vagero, D. (2008). Global health equity and climate stabilisation: a common agenda. Lancet, 372, 1677-1683.
35. Galea, S., & Putnam, S. (2007). The role of macrosocial determinants in shaping the health of populations. In S. Galea (Ed.). Macrosocial determinants of population Health (pp. 3-12). New York: Springer.
36. Glass, T. A. (2006). Commentary: culture in epidemiology - the 800 pound gorilla? International Journal of Epidemiology, 35, 259-261.
37. Grandjean, P., & Landrigan, P. J. (2006). Developmental neurotoxicity of industrial chemicals. Lancet, 368, 2167-2178.
38. Hanlon, P., & Carlisle, S. (2009). Is modern culture bad for our health and wellbeing? Global Health Promotion, 16(4), 27-34.
39. Helliwell, J. F. (2003). How’s life? Combining individual and national variables to explain subjective well-being. Economic Modelling, 20, 331-360.
40. Helliwell, J. F. (2008). Life satisfaction and quality of development. NBER Working Paper Series 14507. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research. http://www.nber.org/papers/w14507. Accessed June 20, 2009.
41. Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., & Layton, J. B. (2010). Social relationships and mortality risk: a meta-analytic review. PLoS Medicine, 7(7): e1000316. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1000316.
42. Horwitz, A. V., & Wakefield, J. C. (2007). The loss of sadness: how psychiatry transformed normal sorrow into depressive disorder. New York: Oxford University Press.
43. Inglehart, R. (2000). Globalization and postmodern values. Washington Quarterly, 23, 215-228.
44. Inglehart, R., Foa, R., Peterson, C., & Welzel, C. (2008). Development, freedom, and rising happiness: a global perspective (1981-2007). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 264-285.
45. Jacka, F. C., Kremer, P. J., Leslie, E.R., et al. (2010). Associations between diet quality and depressed mood in adolescents: results from the Australian Healthy Neighbourhoods Study. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 44, 435-442.
46. Joseph Rowntree Foundation. (2008). What are today’s social evils? York: Joseph Rowntree Foundation. http://www.socialevils.org.uk/documents/social-evils-summary.pdf. Accessed June 20, 2009.
47. Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 593-602.
48. Layard, R., & Dunn, J. (2009). A good childhood: searching for values in a competitive age. Report for the Children’s Society. London: Penguin.
49. McMichael, A. J. (2001). Human frontiers, environments and disease: past patterns, uncertain futures. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
50. McMichael, A. J., Friel, S., Nyong, A., Corvalan, C. (2008). Global environmental change and health: impacts, inequalities, and the health sector. British Medical Journal, 336, 191-194.
51. Mental Health Foundation. (2010). The lonely society. www.mentalhealth.org.uk/campaigns/loneliness-and-mental-health/. Accessed August 19, 2010.
52. Moussavi, S., Chatterji, S., Verdes, E., Tandon, A., Patel, V., & Ustun, B. (2007). Depression, chronic disease, and decrements in health: results from the World Health Surveys. Lancet, 370, 851-858.
53. Moynihan, R., & Henry, D. (2006). The fight against disease mongering: generating knowledge for action. PLoS Medicine, 3, 25-428.
54. National Preventative Health Taskforce. (2009). Australia: the healthiest country by 2020. National preventative health strategy - overview. Canberra: Commonwealth Government of Australia. http://www.preventativehealth.org.au/internet/preventativehealth/publishing.nsf/Content/nphs-overvie w. Accessed September 2, 2009.
55. Nock, M. K., Borges, G., Bromet, E. J., Cha, C. B., Kessler, R. C., & Lee, S. (2008). Suicide and suicidal behavior. Epidemiologic Review, 30, 133-154.
56. Oddy, W.H., Robinson, M., Ambrosini, G.L., et al. (2009). The association between dietary patterns and mental health in early adolescence. Preventive Medicine, 9 (1), 39-44.
57. Oishi, S., & Schimmack, U. Residential mobility, well-being, and mortality. (2010). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(6), 980-994.
58. Ormel, J., Petukhova, M., Chatterji, S., et al. (2008). Disability and treatment of specific mental and physical disorders across the world. British Journal of Psychiatry, 192, 368-375.
59. Parker, G., Gibson, N. A., Brotchie, H., Heruc, G., Rees, A-M., & Hadzi-Pavlovic, D. (2006). Omega-3 fatty acids and mood disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 969-978.
60. Phillips, M. R., Zhang, J., Shi, Q., et al. (2009). Prevalence, treatment, and associated disability of mental disorders in four provinces in China during 2001-05: an epidemiological survey. Lancet, 373, 2041-2053.
61. Popkin, B. M., (2007). The world is fat. Scientific American. 19 August.
62. Prince, M., Patel, V., Saxena, S., et al. (2007). No health without mental health. Lancet, 370, 859-877.
63. Rose, G. (1992). The strategy of preventive medicine. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
64. Schwartz, S., & Diez-Roux, R. (2001). Commentary: causes of incidence and causes of cases – a Durkheimian perspective on Rose. International Journal of Epidemiology, 30, 435-439.
65. Schwarzinger, M., Stouthard, M., Burstrom, K., & Nord, E. (2003). European disability weights group. Cross-national agreement on disability weights: the European disability weights project. Population Health Metrics, 1, 9-20.
66. Selznick, P. (1992). The moral commonwealth: social theory and the promise of community. Berkeley: University of California Press.
67. Sweeting, H., West, P., Young, R., & Der, G. (2010). Can we explain increases in young people’s psychological distress over time? Social Science & Medicine, 71, 1819-1830.
68. Trust for America’s Health. (2008). Prevention for a healthier America: investments in disease prevention yield significant savings, stronger communities. Issue report. Trust for America’s Health, July. http://healthyamericans.org/reports/prevention08/. Accessed June 20, 2009.
69. Twenge, J. M., Gentile, B., DeWall, C. N., Ma, D., Lacefield, K., & Schurtz, D.R. (2010). Birth cohort increases in psychopathology among young Americans, 1938-2007: A cross-temporal meta-analysis of the MMPI. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 145-154.
70. Wade, D. T., & Halligan, P.W. (2004). Do biomedical models of illness make for good healthcare systems? British Medical Journal, 329, 1398-1401.
71. WHO Commission on Social Determinants of Health. (2008). Closing the gap in a generation: health equity through action on the social determinants of health. Final report of the Commission on Social Determinants of Health. Geneva: WHO. http://www.who.int/social_determinants/thecommission/finalreport/en/index.html . Accessed June 20, 2009.
72. WHO. (2001). Mental health: new understanding, new hope. World Health Report 2001. Geneva: World Health Organization.
73. WHO. (2008a). 2008-2013 action plan for the prevention and control of noncommunicable diseases. Geneva: World Health Organization. http://www.who.int/nmh/Actionplan-PC-NCD-2008.pdf . Accessed June 20, 2009.
74. WHO. (2008b). mhGAP: Mental Health Gap Action Programme: scaling up care for mental, neurological and substance use disorders. Geneva: World Health Organization. http://www.who.int/mental_health/mhGAP/en/. Accessed June 20, 2009.
75. WHO. (2008c). The global burden of disease: 2004 update. Geneva: World Health Organization. http://www.who.int/healthinfo/global_burden_disease/2004_report_update/en/index.html. Accessed June 20, 2009.
76. Wilkinson, R., & Pickett, K. (2009). The spirit level: why equality is better for everyone. London: Penguin.
77. Williams, D. R., & Sternthal, M.J. (2007). Spirituality, religion and health: evidence and research directions. Medical Journal of Australia, 186 (10) (suppl), S47-S50.

Source(s) of Funding


No funding.

Competing Interests


No competing interests

Disclaimer


This article has been downloaded from WebmedCentral. With our unique author driven post publication peer review, contents posted on this web portal do not undergo any prepublication peer or editorial review. It is completely the responsibility of the authors to ensure not only scientific and ethical standards of the manuscript but also its grammatical accuracy. Authors must ensure that they obtain all the necessary permissions before submitting any information that requires obtaining a consent or approval from a third party. Authors should also ensure not to submit any information which they do not have the copyright of or of which they have transferred the copyrights to a third party.
Contents on WebmedCentral are purely for biomedical researchers and scientists. They are not meant to cater to the needs of an individual patient. The web portal or any content(s) therein is neither designed to support, nor replace, the relationship that exists between a patient/site visitor and his/her physician. Your use of the WebmedCentral site and its contents is entirely at your own risk. We do not take any responsibility for any harm that you may suffer or inflict on a third person by following the contents of this website.

Comments
0 comments posted so far

Please use this functionality to flag objectionable, inappropriate, inaccurate, and offensive content to WebmedCentral Team and the authors.

 

Author Comments
0 comments posted so far

 

What is article Popularity?

Article popularity is calculated by considering the scores: age of the article
Popularity = (P - 1) / (T + 2)^1.5
Where
P : points is the sum of individual scores, which includes article Views, Downloads, Reviews, Comments and their weightage

Scores   Weightage
Views Points X 1
Download Points X 2
Comment Points X 5
Review Points X 10
Points= sum(Views Points + Download Points + Comment Points + Review Points)
T : time since submission in hours.
P is subtracted by 1 to negate submitter's vote.
Age factor is (time since submission in hours plus two) to the power of 1.5.factor.

How Article Quality Works?

For each article Authors/Readers, Reviewers and WMC Editors can review/rate the articles. These ratings are used to determine Feedback Scores.

In most cases, article receive ratings in the range of 0 to 10. We calculate average of all the ratings and consider it as article quality.

Quality=Average(Authors/Readers Ratings + Reviewers Ratings + WMC Editor Ratings)