Research articles
 

By Dr. Zlatislav Stoyanov , Dr. Ivanka Boncheva , Dr. Piareta Nikolova , Dr. Margarita Ivanova
Corresponding Author Dr. Zlatislav Stoyanov
Department of Physiology and Pathophysiology, Medical University - Varna, Marin Drinov 55 - Bulgaria
Submitting Author Dr. Zlatislav Stoyanov
Other Authors Dr. Ivanka Boncheva
Medical University of Varna, Bulgaria, Department of Psychiatry and Medical Psychology, - Bulgaria

Dr. Piareta Nikolova
Medical University of Varna, Bulgaria, Department of Physiology and Pathophysiology, - Bulgaria

Dr. Margarita Ivanova
Medical University of Varna, Bulgaria, Department of Physiology and Pathophysiology, - Bulgaria

PHYSIOLOGY

Personality, Neuroticism, Mental stress, Cardiovascular stress response

Stoyanov Z, Boncheva I, Nikolova P, Ivanova M. Neuroticism and Vascular Response to Mental Load. WebmedCentral PHYSIOLOGY 2013;4(1):WMC003951
doi: 10.9754/journal.wmc.2013.003951

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License(CC-BY), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
No
Submitted on: 15 Jan 2013 10:44:34 PM GMT
Published on: 16 Jan 2013 02:11:38 PM GMT

Abstract


Various personality traits are thought to have an influence on stress reactivity. The studies aimed at elucidating the association between neuroticism and cardiovascular response to stress have produced contradictory data. The aim of the present study was to compare the vascular responses to mental load in low neurotic and high neurotic subjects. A total of 41 young volunteers (25 males and 16 females) were studied. The Eysenck Personality Inventory was used for assessment of neuroticism. The mental challenge was a combination of memory and mental arithmetic tasks. The values and the reactive changes of the photoplethysmographic index known as “module of elasticity” (ME) were used for assessment of the vascular stress response (higher values of ME indicate higher tonic tension of blood vessels). The ME before mental load had lower values in low neurotic group compared to the high neurotic group: 15.8 vs. 19.3 (t = 2.43; p = 0.02). During the mental load, ME reached higher values in high neurotic group (22.6) in comparison with low neurotics (19.1) (t = 2.00; p = 0.05). Baseline-to-task relative changes in ME were similar in both groups. It can be concluded that an association between neuroticism and anticipatory vascular stress response does exist.

Introduction


Mental stress is an integral part of modern everyday life and the peculiarities of individuals’ stress reactivity can be an important risk factor for health. For instance, exaggerated cardiovascular reactivity is thought to be involved in the pathogenesis of coronary atherosclerosis (1), and is considered as a predictor of essential hypertension (2, 3). There are suggestions that the degree of cardiovascular reactivity to psychological stress may be predictive of later development of cardiovascular disease (4,5).

Various personality traits are thought to have an influence on cardiovascular reactivity (6, 7). The attention of the researchers is focused mainly on personality dimensions closely associated with emotion, because the experience and the regulation of emotions have been associated with cardiovascular responses (8, 9). Neuroticism, one of the Big Five personality traits, is an indicator of proneness to negative affectivity (7). Several studies investigated the relationship between neuroticism and cardiovascular reactivity (10-12), but the results obtained to date are contradictory. Schwebel and Suls (13) found an association of neuroticism with heart rate reactivity to laboratory stressors, but not with blood pressure reactivity. Contrary to that finding, Kennedy and Hughes (14) found an association of neuroticism with blood pressure reactivity, but not with heart rate reactivity. Brody et al. (15) did not find a link between neuroticism and heart rate or blood pressure reactivity in subjects performing a mental arithmetic task. Jonassaint et al. (11) reported that in males neuroticism negatively correlates with vascular reactivity to mental stress. The exact relationship between neuroticism and cardiovascular reactivity has not been clarified yet. That’s why the aim of the present study was to compare the vascular responses to mental load in low neurotic and high neurotic subjects.

Methods


Participants

A total of 41 volunteers (25 males and 16 females), undergraduate students at Varna Medical University, took part in the experiment. Subjects ranged in age from 20 to 27 years. Written informed consent was obtained from subjects after the aim, methods and experimental procedure had been explained.

Assessment of neuroticism and experimental groups formation

The Eysenck Personality Inventory (EPI) was used for assessment of neuroticism. Neuroticism scores were subjected to descriptive statistics and the median was established. According to the median value (10 units) the subjects were divided into two groups: of low neurotic individuals (neuroticism scores below the median) and high neurotic individuals (neuroticism scores above the median) (16, 17). Four subjects with neuroticism scores equal to the median were excluded from sample. Thus the group of low neurotic individuals (mean neuroticism score 6.2) included 17 subjects (13 males and 4 females), and the group of high neurotic individuals (mean neuroticism score 14.2) included 20 subjects (10 males and 10 females).

Physiological data acquisition

The volume changes of peripheral blood flow were monitored and recorded by means of infrared photoplethysmographic (PPG) probe attached by an adhesive collar onto the distal phalanx of the index finger of the participants. The record and processing of the physiological signal were made by using a probe, amplifier, hardware and AcqKnowledge software of Biopac System Inc (CA, USA).

Procedure

Experimental sessions were carried out in the time interval 10 am to 2 pm. The hand on which the PPG-probe was attached was tempered in advance for 10 minutes in water of 36ºC. After the adaptation of the participants to the laboratory environment, in a sitting position, two 2-minute PPG recordings were made: the first one done before the mental load, and the second during mental stress. The laboratory stress was modelled through mental load, which consisted of a short-term memory task for digits combined with a mental arithmetic task (18).

Data reduction and statistical analysis

The physiological data from the second minute of the first record (made before the mental load) and the first minute of the mental load were analyzed, because some of our previous observations showed that the autonomic parameters in these minutes are most informative for the state (19). The parameters of 10 PPG complexes (every 6 seconds) were measured and averaged. The values and reactive changes [(task–baseline)/baseline)] of the photoplethysmographic index known as “module of elasticity” (ME) were used for assessment of the vascular stress response (Figure 1). Higher values of ME indicate higher tonic tension of blood vessels (20, 21). Data were statistically analyzed by means of GraphPad Prism 4.03 software (GraphPad Software, Inc.) An unpaired two-tailed Student’s t-test was used for between-group comparisons. A p<0.05 was considered significant.

Results


The results obtained on ME are presented in Table 1. The values of ME before the mental load had significantly higher values in high neurotic group than in low neurotic: 19.3 vs. 15.8 (t = 2.43; p = 0.02). In high neurotic group the ME was on the upper limit of the physiological range of 13 to 19 relative units (20). During the mental load, the ME increased in both groups reaching higher values in high neurotic group: 22.6 vs. 19.1 in low neurotics (t = 2.00; p = 0.05). Baseline-to-task relative changes in ME were similar in both groups.

Discussion


The results obtained confirmed that neuroticism has an effect on the level of tension of cardiovascular regulatory mechanisms. In laboratory stress conditions, before and during mental stress we established higher vascular tone in the high neurotic group. Our finding corresponds to the opinions that high neurotics have a more reactive sympathetic nervous system (22). Strictly speaking, our results are not in full agreement with literary data suggesting an association of greater reactivity to stress with higher neuroticism (23). According to some definitions, cardiovascular reactivity refers to the magnitude of cardiovascular responses to challenges (4). The magnitude of vascular response (baseline-to-task changes in ME) in our study did not differ between high neurotics and low neurotics. Other psychophysiologists however pay attention to the period preceding the mental task. It can be characterized as a period of emotional, behavioral, and physiological arousal attendant to an impending challenge. According to Everson et al.(1) such elevations in cardiovascular function in anticipation of challenges reflect cardiovascular adjustments in response to psychological and behavioral stresses, and should be considered as a component of cardiovascular reactivity. Thus, the observed significantly higher vascular tone in high neurotics before and during mental load can be assumed to be a manifestation of both exaggerated anticipatory vascular response and vascular reactivity to mental stress.

What kinds of mechanisms underlie the relationship between personality and stress response, and neuroticism and vascular stress reactivity in particular, still remains an unsolved question. According to Flaa et al.(6), the possible mechanisms of psychophysiological reactivity, can be grouped in three main categories: mechanisms at cognitive-emotional level (based on the competencies of the cerebral cortex and limbic system), mechanisms at subcortical level (involving hypothalamic translation of emotional reactions into autonomic or endocrine outputs), and mechanisms at peripheral tissue level (like altered receptor sensitivity). Many researches on the neuroticism provided support for such a point of view. A number of EEG studies have demonstrated that neuroticism is associated with greater activation of the right frontal lobe relative to the left (24,25). It is worth noting that the right hemisphere is more involved both in negative emotion processing (26,27) and in autonomic stress response control (28). In support of the suggested involvement of subcortical mechanisms is the established association of neuroticism with higher levels of adrenocorticotropic hormone and cortisol (7,29). Both stress hormones have cardiovascular effects (30, 31). Cortisol increases sensitivity of adrenergic receptors to norepinephrine and epinephrine, i.e. predisposes to increased cardiovascular reactivity. In this context it is important to mention that neuroticism has been linked to higher levels of norepinephrine (7).

However, the present research was not designed to study the intimate mechanisms underlying the association of neuroticism and cardiovascular stress response. Another limitation of the present study is the small number of the subjects studied. The small sample does not allow us to consider the sex differences established in some of studies on neuroticism and reactivity. We would like to limit our conclusion to the statement that there is an association between neuroticism and tension of cardiovascular regulatory mechanisms, particularly in stress conditions. Further research is needed to clarify the relationships between personality, stress reactivity and health.

References


1. Everson, S. A., Lynch, J. W., Chesney, M. A., Kaplan. G. A., Goldberg. D. E., Shade, S.B., Cohen, R. D., Salonen, R. and Salonen, J. T., Interaction of workplace demands and cardiovascular reactivity in progression of carotid atherosclerosis: population based study. BMJ, 314:553-558, 1997.
2. Kasagi, F., Akahoshi, M. and Shimaoka, K., Relation between cold pressor test and development of hypertension based on 28-year follow-up. Hypertension, 25:71-76, 1995.
3. Schwartz, C. E., Durocher, J. J. and Carter, J. R., Neurovascular responses to mental stress in prehypertensive humans. J Appl Physiol, 110(1):76-82, 2011.
4. Andreassi, J. L., The Psychophysiology of cardiovascular reactivity. Int J Psychophysiol, 25(1):7-11, 1997.
5. Farag, N. H., Vincent, A. S., McKey, B. S., Al'Absi, M., Whitsett, T. L. and Lovallo W. R., Sex differences in the hemodynamic responses to mental stress: Effect of caffeine consumption. Psychophysiology, 43(4): 337-343, 2006.
6. Flaa, A., Ekeberg, Ø., Kjeldsen, S. E. and Rostrup, M., Personality may influence reactivity to stress. Biopsychosoc Med, 1:5, doi: 10.1186/1751-0759-1-5, 2007.
7. DeYoung ,C. G. and Gray, J. R., Personality neuroscience: explaining individual differences in affect, behavior, and cognition. In: Corr P. J., Matthews G. (eds), Cambridge handbook of personality. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 323-346, 2009.
8. Gross, J. J., Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. J Pers Soc Psychol, 74:224-237, 1998.
9. Herrald, M. M. and Tomaka, J., Patterns of emotion-specific appraisal, coping, and cardiovascular reactivity during an ongoing emotional episode. J Pers Soc Psychol, 83:434-450, 2002.
10. Schneider, T. R., The role of neuroticism on psychological and physiological stress responses. J Exp Soc Psychol, 40(6):795-804, 2004.
11. Jonassaint, C. R., Why, Y. P., Bishop, G. D., Tong, E. M., Diong, S. M., Enkelmann, H. C., Khader, M. and Ang, J., The effects of neuroticism and extraversion on cardiovascular reactivity during a mental and an emotional stress task. Int J Psychophysiol, 74 (3):274-279, 2009.
12. Hutchinson,  J. G. and Ruiz, J. M., Neuroticism and cardiovascular response in women: evidence of effects on blood pressure recovery. J Pers, 79(2):277-302, 2011.
13. Schwebel, D. C. and Suls, J., Cardiovascular reactivity and neuroticism: results from a laboratory and controlled ambulatory stress protocol. J Pers, 67(1):67-92, 1999.
14. Kennedy, D.K. and Hughes, B.M., The optimism-neuroticism question: An evaluation based on cardiovascular reactivity in female college students. Psychol Rec, 54:373-386, 2004.
15. Brody, S., Veit, R. and Rau, H., Neuroticism but not cardiovascular stress reactivity is associated with less longitudinal blood pressure increase. Pers Indiv Differ, 20(3):375-380, 1996.
16. Barret, J. and Armony, J., The influence of trait anxiety on autonomic response and cognitive performance during an anticipatory anxiety task. Depress Anxiety, 23:210-219, 2006.
17. Gramer, M. and Saria, K., Effects of social anxiety and evaluative threat on cardiovascular responses to active performance situations. Biol Psychol, 74:67-74, 2007.
18. Piryov, G. and Tsanev, T., Experimental Psychology. Nauka i Izkustvo, Sofia, 1973. (in Bulgarian)
19. Stoyanov, Z., Marinov, M. and Radev, R., Peripheral vascular response to mental load in left- and right-handers. Int J Psychophysiol, 61:358, 2006.
20. Bersenev, V., Guba, G. and Pyatak, O., Handbook of Clinical Neurovegetology. Zdorovya, Kiev, 1990. (in Russian)
21. Oskolkova, M., Functional Methods of Investigation of Circulatory System in Children. Medicina, Moscow, 1988. (in Russian)
22. Weibel, D., Wissmath, B. and Stricker, D., The influence of neuroticism on spatial presence and enjoyment in films. Pers Indiv Differ, 51(7):866-869, 2011.
23. Siegman, A. W., Anderson, R. A. and Berger, T., The angry voice: its effects on the experience of anger and cardiovascular reactivity. Psychosom Med, 52: 631-643, 1990.
24. Davidson, R. J., Anxiety and affective style: role of prefrontal cortex and amygdala. Biol Psychiatry, 51:68-80, 2002.
25. Zuckerman, M., Psychobiology of personality (2nd ed., revised and updated). Cambridge University Press, New York, 2005.
26. Davidson, R. J., Cerebral asymmetry, emotion and affective style. In: Davidson R. J., Hugdahl K. (eds), Brain Asymmetry. A Bradford Book, The MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, London, England, pp 361-387, 1995.
27. Demaree, H. A., Everhart, D. E., Youngstrom, E. A. and Harrison, D. W., Brain lateralization of emotional processing: historical roots and a future incorporating "Dominance". Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev, 4(1):3-20, 2005.
28. Wittling, W., The right hemisphere and the human stress response. In: Folkow B., Schmidt T. and Uvnäs-Moberg K. (eds) Stress, health, and the social environment. Acta Physiol Scand, Suppl. 640:55-59, 1997.
29. Portella, M. J., Harmer, C. J., Flint, J., Cowen, P. and Goodwin, G. M., Enhanced Early Morning Salivary Cortisol in Neuroticism. Am J Psychiatry, 162:807-809, 2005.
30. Berne, R., Levy, M., Koeppen, B. and Stanton, B., Physiology (fifth edition), Elsevier, Mosby, 2004.
31. Hatakeyama, H., Inaba, S., Taniguchi, N. and Miyamori, I., Functional adrenocorticotropic hormone receptor in cultured human vascular endothelial cells: possible role in control of blood pressure. Hypertension, 36:862-865, 2000.

Source(s) of Funding


None

Competing Interests


None

Disclaimer


This article has been downloaded from WebmedCentral. With our unique author driven post publication peer review, contents posted on this web portal do not undergo any prepublication peer or editorial review. It is completely the responsibility of the authors to ensure not only scientific and ethical standards of the manuscript but also its grammatical accuracy. Authors must ensure that they obtain all the necessary permissions before submitting any information that requires obtaining a consent or approval from a third party. Authors should also ensure not to submit any information which they do not have the copyright of or of which they have transferred the copyrights to a third party.
Contents on WebmedCentral are purely for biomedical researchers and scientists. They are not meant to cater to the needs of an individual patient. The web portal or any content(s) therein is neither designed to support, nor replace, the relationship that exists between a patient/site visitor and his/her physician. Your use of the WebmedCentral site and its contents is entirely at your own risk. We do not take any responsibility for any harm that you may suffer or inflict on a third person by following the contents of this website.

Comments
0 comments posted so far

Please use this functionality to flag objectionable, inappropriate, inaccurate, and offensive content to WebmedCentral Team and the authors.

 

Author Comments
0 comments posted so far

 

What is article Popularity?

Article popularity is calculated by considering the scores: age of the article
Popularity = (P - 1) / (T + 2)^1.5
Where
P : points is the sum of individual scores, which includes article Views, Downloads, Reviews, Comments and their weightage

Scores   Weightage
Views Points X 1
Download Points X 2
Comment Points X 5
Review Points X 10
Points= sum(Views Points + Download Points + Comment Points + Review Points)
T : time since submission in hours.
P is subtracted by 1 to negate submitter's vote.
Age factor is (time since submission in hours plus two) to the power of 1.5.factor.

How Article Quality Works?

For each article Authors/Readers, Reviewers and WMC Editors can review/rate the articles. These ratings are used to determine Feedback Scores.

In most cases, article receive ratings in the range of 0 to 10. We calculate average of all the ratings and consider it as article quality.

Quality=Average(Authors/Readers Ratings + Reviewers Ratings + WMC Editor Ratings)